A study conducted at the University of São Paulo analyzed how dogs living inside the home exchange looks with their owners to obtain food or other desired objects, comparing them with those who live outside the home and in shelters (photo: researchers’ archive)

Life experience shapes dogs’ interaction with humans
2021-11-17
PT ES

A study conducted at the University of São Paulo analyzed how dogs living inside the home exchange looks with their owners to obtain food or other desired objects, comparing them with those who live outside the home and in shelters.

Life experience shapes dogs’ interaction with humans

A study conducted at the University of São Paulo analyzed how dogs living inside the home exchange looks with their owners to obtain food or other desired objects, comparing them with those who live outside the home and in shelters.

2021-11-17
PT ES

A study conducted at the University of São Paulo analyzed how dogs living inside the home exchange looks with their owners to obtain food or other desired objects, comparing them with those who live outside the home and in shelters (photo: researchers’ archive)

 

By Maria Fernanda Ziegler  |  Agência FAPESP – It’s an everyday scene. Pooch wants a tasty-looking piece of cake that’s sitting on the table. What does he do? He gazes longingly at the titbit, ogles his master or mistress, and glances back at the cake. This “conversation” with humans gets him what he wants.

Communication between dogs and humans involving an exchange of looks is very common, but different life experiences can change it, according to a study conducted at the University of São Paulo (USP) in Brazil.

The study was supported by FAPESP via a project to develop an ethological approach to social communication between various species – humans included.

An article on the study is published in the journal Behavioural Processes, discussing the use of gaze alternation by dogs. Gaze alternation can be defined as staring at a human to get their attention, flicking the eyes to where an unattainable object is located, then gazing back at the human, all with little or no head movement.

The researchers compared 60 pet dogs of various breeds and ages. They found that 95.7% of those living inside the home used gaze alternation at least once, while those living outside communicated less intensely (80%). Dogs living in shelters had less contact with humans and interacted less (58.8%).

“The findings point to strong influence of life experience on development and use of communicative behavior. Dogs that spent longer near people were more disposed to use communication as a strategy to obtain a desired objective,” said Juliana Wallner Werneck Mendes, who conducted the experiment at the Canine Laboratory in USP’s Department of Psychology during her master’s research.

This is the first study on the difference between dogs that live alongside humans inside the home and those that live outside and interact less intensely with their owners.

“Another important aspect we observed was that all groups communicated,” Mendes noted. “Shelter dogs used to be considered unable to communicate with humans. In fact, they do, but less so. This shows that the experiences of a lifetime result in different kinds of behavior.”

According to Mendes, low interaction by shelter dogs should not be interpreted as incapacity. “On the contrary, they’re capable of communicating, even with little exposure to humans,” she said. “Previous studies have shown that they learn to exchange looks very quickly when interacting with humans.”

This capacity derives from the animal’s ability to learn. “Shelter dogs are very well-adapted to the situation. They don’t need to communicate with humans for much of their lives,” said Briseida de Resende, a professor at USP and Mendes’s joint thesis advisor with Carine Savalli Redigolo

Resende explained that the findings from the study refute an old dichotomy between nature and nurture in the field of ethology, the science of animal behavior. “Canine behavior is largely inherited, of course, and they were domesticated a very long time ago, but this aspect should never be abstracted from the context in which they live. Nurture and the environment are also very important,” she said. “The micro context [life experience] is as influential as the macro [evolution of the species]. The nature versus nurture debate has been going on for a long time, and we’re always looking to see how much canine behavior is innate and how much is learned, but currently we’re moving toward the view that it makes no sense to separate the two.”

The article “Effect of different experiences with humans in dogs’ visual communication” (doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2021.104487) by Juliana Wallner Werneck Mendes, Briseida Resende and Carine Savalli is at: www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/abs/pii/S0376635721001716#!.

 

  Republish
 

Republish

The Agency FAPESP licenses news via Creative Commons (CC-BY-NC-ND) so that they can be republished free of charge and in a simple way by other digital or printed vehicles. Agência FAPESP must be credited as the source of the content being republished and the name of the reporter (if any) must be attributed. Using the HMTL button below allows compliance with these rules, detailed in Digital Republishing Policy FAPESP.